Family Experiences of Vegetative and Minimally Conscious States

What is a ‘coma’ and ‘what is a ‘vegetative state'?

COMA

In the media and in everyday speech the word ‘coma’ is sometimes used in a general way to cover a wide range of conditions in which the individual has suffered brain injury, leaving them with no consciousness at all or with very limited consciousness. We read about celebrities, sports personalities or politicians said to be in a ‘coma’ after months or years but they are probably in a vegetative or minimally conscious state. It’s very unusual for a coma to last more than a few weeks at most. People in a coma are completely unresponsive. They do not move, do not react to light or sound and cannot feel pain. Their eyes are closed. The brain responds to extreme trauma by effectively ‘shutting down’. Some scientists compare it to being under a general anaesthetic. Sometimes patients with very severe brain injuries who do not enter a coma naturally are placed in a medically-induced coma (using a drug like propofol which is used in anaesthesia) to try to allow the brain time to heal. After a few days or weeks in a coma a person who does not die usually ‘wakes up’ in the sense that their eyes open. If they have only been in a coma for a few days they may ‘wake up’ to full consciousness with relatively little damage. If the person has very severe brain injuries though, they may move from coma into a vegetative or minimally conscious state.

VEGETATIVE STATE 

In a vegetative state the person is still unconscious. They have no awareness of themselves or their environment. The main difference between ‘coma’ and the ‘vegetative state’ is that at some point the person’s eyes will be open and there will be times when they seem to be ‘awake’. They may move parts of their body, but this movement is not voluntary. Movements can include grinding their teeth, thrashing, and facial movements such as grimacing, yawning or smiling. They might jerk as a reflex response to loud noises or move a hand away from a source of pain. They may produce sounds (grunting or moaning) or even occasional words. After four weeks the person is said to be in a ‘prolonged’ vegetative state. If a person remains in a vegetative state for several months after brain damage involving oxygen-deprivation (caused, for example, by a cardiac arrest), or for one year after a traumatic brain injury (such as a car accident, a fall, or an assault), the chances of recovering consciousness are very low and they are said to be in a “permanent” vegetative state. 

Many people dislike hearing the word ‘vegetative’ applied to someone they love. There are other names for the condition (‘apallic syndrome’ or ‘unresponsive wakefulness syndrome’) – but the ‘vegetative state’ is still the most widely used term in the UK.

Imogen talks about ‘that horrible expression - the permanently vegetative state’ which was her husband’s diagnosis.

Imogen talks about ‘that horrible expression - the permanently vegetative state’ which was her husband’s diagnosis.

SHOW TEXT VERSION
PRINT TRANSCRIPT
And that horrible expression, “the permanently vegetative state” is… I’m pleased it’s just… A coma is kind of a nice word and you envisage something - a bubble around someone. But a “permanent vegetative state” reminds me of something like a cauliflower - that you've become something moldy, a moldy vegetable for some reason.

So you think it fits?

Yes, it does.

Because some people hate that, they don’t want their loved ones described as vegetables. 

No, I don’t like it. (It’s thundering now!) I didn’t like it. No, I'm not saying- It fits. But once you accept that that's what it is, then you know how horrific it is. But you don’t want the whole world to think of your - the person they knew as vibrant and energetic as a vegetable.

Professor Derick Wade of the Oxford Centre for Enablement talks about the meaning of the words “coma” and the “vegetative state”.

Professor Derick Wade of the Oxford Centre for Enablement talks about the meaning of the words “coma” and the “vegetative state”.

SHOW TEXT VERSION
PRINT TRANSCRIPT
Coma is really the word that’s used to refer to someone being unresponsive, unconscious – a plain English term just meaning they’re not responding at all. We assume that they’re unaware of anything that’s happened. They will not talk. They will not move. They will not do anything at all. They may need ventilation support or they may not. But they’re unresponsive. And they will remain in that state, with their eyes closed, throughout. So one of the things that defines coma is that your eyes are closed. At some point, usually within a week, two weeks, if they remain in that state, they will start to open their eyes. They will start to be slightly more reactive and responsive. And at some point they will move into a state where they are termed often “vegetative”. All that means, really, is that having lost the sleep-wake cycle- For the first week, two weeks, they don’t wake up and go to sleep with their eyes opening and closing, they just stay the same, they start to have a time when they appear more awake and a time when they appear more asleep. Their eyes will be open. Their eyes might be moving around. They might be twitching or moving a little bit. And in that case you might refer to that – or people do refer to that – as the vegetative state. And it really just means that the functions of the body of pulse, blood pressure, breathing, digesting, which are known as ‘vegetative’ functions, are working and are going along in the normal way. But they are still not with us, they’re still not aware, they’re still not responding in a meaningful way to what’s happening around them. So it’s slightly artificial because from the point of view of the outside person they’re still unaware, they’re still essentially not responding. But in a descriptive sense we call them vegetative.
Questions are often raised about whether you can be sure that someone definitely has no awareness of themselves or their environment. Here Derick Wade addresses this question.

How can you be sure that someone is not conscious? Derick Wade describes how he assesses whether or not a patient is conscious.

How can you be sure that someone is not conscious? Derick Wade describes how he assesses whether or not a patient is conscious.

SHOW TEXT VERSION
PRINT TRANSCRIPT
How can you be sure that a patient is vegetative and has no awareness of themselves or the outside world?

Right, well in terms of being sure that someone is unaware of themselves, in the end that’s a human clinical judgment. So there is no electrical test or scanning test that will tell you definitively that this person is aware or not aware. So the way that I do it and most people do it is to look at somebody’s behavior – in other words the way they move, the way their face moves, their hands move, their arms move, whatever it might be, and to say, “Does that behavior require the person to be aware, or is that entirely a reflex?”. So has that person extracted some meaning from what is happening? So if the bell goes, if a telephone bell goes and somebody reaches out as if to pick up a telephone and as if to put it to their ear, they’ve obviously understood that it’s a bell, which means telephone, and they obviously know that a telephone is something that you pick up with your hand. If the bell goes and they just jump, they’ve heard the noise, but they don’t take any meaning from it. They’d do the same if I clapped my hands, rang a bell, opened the window, probably, with a loud bang. So they’ve got to get some meaning out of it. And in terms of their movements, if they’re doing something that implies they have an abstract goal. So if I pinch somebody on their hand to see how they respond to pain, and they withdraw their hand, that’s a reflex. If I pinch anybody, if I pinch you, you would withdraw your hand – among other things. If I pinch someone who’s conscious they will also try to take my hand away, hit me, they’ll do a whole lot of other things. Which imply that they’ve worked out that somebody is doing something and they want to stop it. So if somebody was apparently in a coma, or apparently in the vegetative state, and I pinched their hand on the left, and the right hand comes across, catches hold of my hand, and pulls it away, clearly that means they’re aware. They were aware that somebody, me, was causing them pain and they thought how can they remove it. So in the end, saying somebody is vegetative means that you have determined that nothing that they do requires them to have formulated a goal, formulated an idea, or understood the meaning of what is happening around them. And that does come down to commonsense, in a way. You have to decide that. Because people in the vegetative state will respond to noise - by startle or jump, will respond to pain – by moving away, often will have some spontaneous movement, their eyes will move all over the place, they might yawn, sometimes they might stretch or they might move their arms. Rarely do they do a lot of movement – I have seen them, but that’s rare. But they’re not immobile or unresponsive. It’s the nature of the response that determines reflex against awareness.
Another key concern is whether a vegetative patient can feel pain. Some people report that the patient shows no signs of pain. Morag, for example, said of her father that while he was in a vegetative state,  “he was in what we could call a deep coma, where you know, if you’d have put pins in him he wouldn’t have reacted at all.”  Sometimes however patients seem to show facial expressions or reactions to painful stimulus that make families worry that they are experiencing pain.

Can a vegetative patient feel pain?

Can a vegetative patient feel pain?

SHOW TEXT VERSION
PRINT TRANSCRIPT
The question of whether someone in the vegetative state can feel pain is interesting and difficult to answer. I personally believe that they are not aware of pain. If you could ask them I don’t think that they would say that they feel pain. And of course we talk about it – we say they feel pain and withdraw. What I should be saying is, there’s a painful stimulus, which reaches the central nervous system – probably the spinal cord – up a nerve that normally carries pain, that causes a reflex movement in the spinal cord to move the hand away. I’m implying, when I say they’re not aware, that it is not getting up to the brain, to a part of the brain that says “I (whoever I am) am feeling pain”. So they don’t feel it. But they respond to it.

Fern does not think her partner feels pain

Fern does not think her partner feels pain

SHOW TEXT VERSION
PRINT TRANSCRIPT
And I think it’s the appearance as well, it’s the appearance of suffering. I call it the appearance of suffering and I won’t call it suffering, because it’s not, it’s the appearance of suffering. From eyes watching and looking at another body, that body is suffering. But he’s not in there to suffer, I tell you that now. He’s not feeling any of it. So it might look like, every time he goes through these.

When he goes through these phases of illness, he’s not feeling it because he’s not in there. He’s not, it’s the appearance. 

The body is going through something, but he is not in that body, he does not feel it. 

And we have proof of that, do you know, it’s things like – there are certain clinical signs when someone is in pain. [Partner’s name] does not show them. 

And it’s not just that – he’s on loads of pain med anyway. So even if people did believe he’s in there – again it’s the appearance of suffering, because he’s on so much pain medicine. It’s lovely [laughs], I feel really good about it, he’s not feeling it, he’s not feeling it. I’m absolutely certain of it, absolutely certain of it. He does not feel any of it. 

Fern describes how her partner did appear to be suffering before he was given ongoing long-term pain relief. Since he has been administered pain relief she has noticed that he appears more relaxed and not in pain.

Fern describes how her partner did appear to be suffering before he was given ongoing long-term pain relief. Since he has been administered pain relief she has noticed that he appears more relaxed and not in pain.

SHOW TEXT VERSION
PRINT TRANSCRIPT
So he wasn’t on long-term pain relief. He was on, as and when paracetamol [laughs]. 

Anyway, but do you know that he was on – so this was the first time we’ve had actual pain relief and it does make a difference. And I can see it, because he just relaxes. 

That dystonic arm thing – he come down. The breathing, it regulates. And now the thing is, you can do the SATS level, and it still may be done at ninety, or eighty-nine or whatever. It’s not gained anymore. Sometimes it was even lower, we’ve had it down at eighty. But he’s not suffering, right? I’ve seen him suffer [deep breath]. The sweat, the panic, you know, we had him sat up and he was just not coping. Do you know, his face was panicked, he couldn’t sleep, he wasn’t resting. He was puffing, he was puffing, he was gurgling. The heart rate was shot up, do you know, it was uncomfortable. 

Now that – he can clearly not be getting much oxygen in, he’s not getting much oxygen and yet he’s so much more peaceful. It’s very obvious that he is resting, you know, and he’s just – he’s okay with that. He’s not – he may not be getting it, but he doesn’t seem to be feeling that.

I can’t explain what he’s feeling, I just know when I see him, I know the difference. I know when he’s uncomfortable because everything changes. He’s – it’s like he’s fighting for something. Fighting for oxygen, fighting to get stuff in. His eyes are always searching for something and he just doesn’t seem to be – he’s fine. I mean that blood pressure, that heart rate is normal. 
Here some different families talk about their experience of their relative.

David and Olivia describe how they experienced being with David’s mother when she was in a ‘permanent vegetative state’.

David and Olivia describe how they experienced being with David’s mother when she was in a ‘permanent vegetative state’.

SHOW TEXT VERSION
PRINT TRANSCRIPT
Olivia: It wasn’t her anymore.

David: No.

Olivia: It was her body. It wasn’t her. But I suppose bizarrely, maybe as a daughter-in-law, out of respect for my mother-in-law, you know, I would – I treated her as I would have done if she would have been—

David: Well, yeah, yeah.

Olivia: her normal self.

David: But yeah, but – I suppose, you know, obviously no communication, disfigurement body wise, rolling of the eyes, things like that just – you know, it was just – it was a shell basically with a few functions going on inside it and everything else being automated. And whenever – like I say, whenever you were with her the machines were in [makes machine noises] you know, everything going on and bleeping and so on, so she – you know, she was there, in appearance you could still see her, and especially when she was asleep you could still see her. But when she woke or whatever—

Olivia: She had her eyes open.

David: Yeah, when she had her eyes open. She – you know, she wasn’t there. She wasn’t there. You’re – like I say, in the early days you were looking – I looked so hard, I wanted to find – I wanted to find I think in my head, I wanted to find something, and I would make – you know, not make anything up but you would look deep in her eyes. You know, no one else in the room, just holding her hand, you know, is there? Is there? And never, ever saw even a glimpse. So you sort of, you know, came to the conclusion that, you know, like I say, it – there was nothing – no coming back basically from what, what had happened. 

Mark’s brother has still not received a formal diagnosis, three years after he was hit by a car while cycling. They are not sure whether he is ‘vegetative’ or minimally conscious’ and, watching him, they wonder if he experiences emotions.

Mark’s brother has still not received a formal diagnosis, three years after he was hit by a car while cycling. They are not sure whether he is ‘vegetative’ or minimally conscious’ and, watching him, they wonder if he experiences emotions.

SHOW TEXT VERSION
PRINT TRANSCRIPT
Mark: He has a smile on his face at times, which seems fairly random. He cries sometimes, which is quite distressing. One of your daughters had a bad time with that once, didn’t they? And that seems to be spontaneous and then - . Oh, it’s one of – like a lot of things with him, he – I can be – I can persuade myself one way or the other about some of these things and so can the medical people. And when it happened in hospital they used to say, “It’s just his eyes watering and you don’t want to take any notice of that really.” But when you’re looking at somebody and they’re looking straight in your eyes and they start crying, you can't not take any notice of that, I don’t care what anybody says really. And if that coincides with something that you’re talking about, that you suddenly think, well, actually he might find that a bit distressing, it does make you think a bit more.

For more on family observations of being with the patient, see ‘Impact of visiting

​Last reviewed December 2017.

Copyright © 2024 University of Oxford. All rights reserved.